Skip to main content

Is the association between optimistic cardiovascular risk perceptions and lower rates of cardiovascular disease mortality explained by biomarkers of systemic inflammation or endothelial function? A case-cohort study

Abstract

Background

More optimistic perceptions of cardiovascular disease risk are associated with substantively lower rates of cardiovascular death among men. It remains unknown whether this association represents causality (i.e. perception leads to actions/conditions that influence cardiovascular disease occurrence) or residual confounding by unmeasured factors that associate with risk perceptions and with physiological processes that promote cardiovascular disease (i.e. inflammation or endothelial dysfunction).

Purpose

To evaluate whether previously unmeasured biological markers of inflammation or endothelial dysregulation confound the observed association between cardiovascular disease risk perceptions and cardiovascular disease outcomes;

Methods

We conducted a nested case-cohort study among community-dwelling men from Southeastern New England (USA) who were interviewed between 1989 and 1990 as part of the Pawtucket Heart Health Program. We measured C-reactive protein (CRP) and Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (VEGF) levels from stored sera for a random sample of the parent cohort (control sample, n = 127) and all cases of cardiovascular death observed through 2005 (case sample, n = 44). We evaluated potential confounding using stratified analyses and logistic regression modeling.

Results

Optimistic ratings of risk associated with lower odds of dying from cardiovascular causes among men (OR = 0.39, 95% CI = 0.17, 0.91). Neither CRP nor VEGF confounded these findings.

Conclusions

The strong cardio-protective association between optimistic ratings of cardiovascular disease risk and lower rates of cardiovascular mortality among men is not confounded by baseline biomarkers of systemic inflammation or endothelial dysfunction.

Background

Optimistic perceptions of cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk are common [1–7] and often targeted by public health risk communication campaigns in hopes of changing CVD risk behavior (i.e. raising perception of CVD risk to motivate heart healthy actions)[7]. However, a growing body of work observes that optimistic perceptions of CVD risk predict lower rates of myocardial infarction [8–10], thus raising the question of whether seeking to overcome these "optimistic biases" will benefit or harm public health.

Considering plausible causal mechanisms via which optimistic biases might act to prevent heart disease is important; however, we must also thoroughly examine whether our prior observations of the CVD risk perception--outcome association are, in fact, confounded by unmeasured CVD risk factors. Risk perceptions are plausibly influenced by psychological conditions (i.e. personality, mood, coping style) that influence inflammatory status and endothelial reactivity. Since biomarkers of systemic inflammation (C-reactive protein) and endothelial dysregulation (Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor) also predict CVD events [11–16], these physiologic processes represent important pathways for potential confounding of the risk perception--CVD outcome association (see conceptual diagram in Figure 1). We hypothesize that controlling for markers of systemic inflammation and endothelial dysregulation will weaken earlier observations of a strong protective association between optimistic ratings of CVD risk and 15-year CVD mortality among community-dwelling men [9].

Figure 1
figure 1

Hypothetical sources of unmeasured confounding and adjustment for CRP and VEGF. E = Comparative optimism in heart disease risk. O = CVD death. C = Systemic inflammation/endothelial dysfunction. U = Unmeasured potential confounder (e.g. personality, mood) A Directed Acyclic Graph (DAG) is a useful methodological tool for considering bias and confounding in complex associations. The DAG shown above in Figure 1 graphically represents how measures of systemic inflammation and endothelial dysfunction, C, relate to a set of plausible, yet unmeasured psychological confounders, U (i.e. mood, self-rated health, and personality) of the relation between comparatively optimistic ratings of CVD risk, E, and CVD death, O. Under hypothetical conditions where unmeasured psychological factors contributed to substantial residual confounding, adjusting for C (as this study does) blocks major physiological pathways that would be responsible for such residual confounding by U.

Methods

Overview

We conducted a case-cohort study nested within a community-based sample of men from southeastern New England (USA). A case-cohort study is an efficient epidemiologic design similar to a case-control study, except that the control group is chosen without regard to the outcome (i.e. a random sample of the base population), thus allowing comparison to multiple "case" groups (e.g. cardiovascular death, as in the current analysis). This strategy averts the time and financial cost of analyzing frozen sera from a full cohort.

All participants completed household interviews between 1990 and 1993. Serum samples were obtained during household interviews and frozen per standard protocol. Death record information was obtained from the National Death Index through December 2005 to identify cases. In depth description of this nested case-cohort study [16] and the parent cohort [9] from which participants were sampled has been published elsewhere.

Participants and eligibility

All participants were enrolled during the post-intervention period of the Pawtucket Heart Health Program (PHHP), a controlled community intervention designed to lower prevalence of CVD risk factors [17]. One thousand one hundred seventy four men from the intervention and control cities who were between the ages of 40 and 74 years and had no personal history of myocardial infarction completed household interviews in the years immediately following the PHHP intervention (1989-1991). From this base population, sera were analyzed for all 44 cases of cardiovascular death identified through 2005 (see description below) and for a random sub-cohort of 127 men that was selected without regard to outcome. The random sub-cohort included 9 of the 44 cases.

Exposure definition

Participants were asked at the beginning of the household interview, "Compared with persons of your own age and sex, how would you rate your risk of having a heart attack or stroke within the next 5 years?" Participants rated their CVD risk as "high", "average", "low", or "don't know". Endorsing one's risk to be lower than average we refer to as an optimistic rating.

Case definition and selection

We obtained the date of death and underlying cause of death by linking personal identifiers (first name, middle initial, last name, sex, exact day/month/year of birth) for each participant to the Center for Disease Control National Death Index (NDI) for the years 1990-2005. Linkage and cause of death were established using NDI-standardized protocols. Cases were those participants whose primary or contributing cause of death was due to cardiovascular disease, as defined by International Classification of Disease (ICD) codes for coronary heart disease or stroke (ICD-9 codes: 410-414 and 430-438; ICD 10 codes: 120-125 and 160-169).

Biomarker measurement

Using thawed sera, the concentrations of CRP and VEGF were measured using immunoturbidimetric and ELISA assays, respectively. Prior epidemiologic research has verified the validity of these samples and measurement procedures [16].

Confounding and Effect Measure Modification

We considered the following as potential confounders or effect measure modifiers of the association between comparably optimistic self-rated CVD risk and CVD mortality: CRP, VEGF, age, race/ethnicity, income, education, foreign birth, city, Framingham Risk Score, total/HDL cholesterol ratio, hypertension, smoking status, body mass index, 1st degree family history of early-onset coronary heart disease, current use of lipid or blood pressure lowering medications, current aspirin use, physical inactivity, alcohol, and psychotropic medications.

Analyses

We evaluated the frequency and distribution of key study variables. For univariate comparisons shown in Table 1, we used the chi-square (categorical variables) and the Student's t-test (continuous variables) for determining statistical significance. Both VEGF and CRP demonstrated substantially skewed distributions with long right-sided tails. Thus, for all analyses considering these biomarkers as continuous variables, we perform natural logarithm transformations. Next, we evaluated potential confounder status for both CRP and VEGF by examining their association with optimistic ratings of risk in the random sub-cohort, since this represents the source population from which cases arise. Similarly, we examined whether CRP and VEGF were associated with CVD mortality, using the full case-cohort sample. We performed similar analyses for other potential confounding variables.

Table 1 Description of Sample by Case Status

Since more elaborate adjustment for potential social, biologic and behavioral confounders (propensity score analysis [18] and censoring of early follow-up time) did not improve upon age and Framingham Heart Score adjustment in the parent cohort [9], we limited adjustment methods here to multivariate logistic regression modeling.

We used SAS software (Cary, NC) for all statistical analyses. Where p-values are reported, we used a statistical significance threshold of 0.05.

The institutional research review boards at Memorial Hospital of Rhode Island and the University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry approved this study.

Results

Thirty one percent of the participants endorsed their risk of having a cardiovascular event in the next five years as lower than average for their age and sex (i.e. optimistic ratings). Forty-four participants died of cardiovascular causes during the 15-year surveillance period. Higher levels of CRP and VEGF were associated with higher risk of CVD mortality (see Table 1). Differences in log-transformed mean levels of VEGF (5.51 versus 5.38; p = 0.37) and CRP (0.56 versus 0.52; p = 0.83) for comparative optimists compared to others were small and statistically insignificant.

The association between comparative optimism and a lower probability of CVD death was strong (OR = 0.39, 95% CI = 0.17, 0.91). Adjusting for CRP alone, VEGF alone, or in combination with age, income and Framingham Risk Score did not substantially change these findings [see Table 2]. Similar results were observed for biomarker classification as a continuous (log-transformed) or categorical (tertile) variable.

Table 2 Crude and adjusted association between comparative optimism and CVD mortality

Discussion and Conclusions

Following previous findings of an independent association between optimistic perceptions of CVD risk and substantially lower rates of CVD mortality among men, we evaluated whether biomarkers of stress (i.e. inflammation and endothelial dysregulation) would confound these provocative observations. We observed that controlling for baseline CRP and VEGF did not weaken the protective association between optimistic ratings of CVD risk and CVD mortality.

Psychosocial and behavioral processes related to self-enhancement and optimism might explain our findings. Believing that one is invulnerable from future problems may enhance motivation to pursue other activities that, in turn, buffer the effect of stress on CVD risk. In addition, people who perceive lower risk may also plan to take measures to reduce risk, making their perceptions accurate.

Another possible explanation is that optimistic ratings of CVD risk reflect an optimistic personality. Optimists seem to experience better health than non-optimists. Among cardiac patients, optimists show faster recovery from cardiac bypass surgery [19] and are less likely to be hospitalized 6 months post-surgery [20]. The beneficial health effects of optimism may arise from the different ways that optimists and pessimists cope with stress. Optimism is correlated with active and problem-focused coping strategies [21] that may lead to better health practices. Research has found that optimists do seem to be more effective in changing their own health behaviors and they report engaging in more healthy behaviors than do pessimists [22]. For example, when compared with pessimistic patients, optimistic cardiac patients were more successful in lowering their fat intake and getting more exercise [23]. However, prior work observes that the correlation between optimistic risk perceptions and global optimism is weak, ranging from r = 0.14 to r = 0.33 [24], thus suggesting that optimistic appraisal of one's CVD risk represents a sub-domain of optimism with potentially important cardio-protective implications.

Fiscella [25] raises important questions about whether optimistic ratings of health status--well known to predict survival [26]-- confound the cardio-protective association with optimistic self-ratings of CVD risk observed in this cohort. Health status was not measured, thus precluding us from directly addressing confounding (or interaction between optimistic ratings of health status and risk). However, optimistic self-rated health is associated with lower levels of systemic inflammation [27, 28]. We observed no such relationship between optimistic ratings of CVD risk and systemic inflammation. Furthermore, controlling for systemic inflammation failed to weaken the observed cardio-protective effects of optimistic ratings of CVD risk. Thus, we conclude that unmeasured confounding by self-rated health status is unlikely to threaten the validity of the strong relationship observed in the present study.

Our findings do not rule out the possibility for systemic inflammation or endothelial dysregulation to represent a partial causal mechanism linking risk perceptions to CVD mortality. For example, if heightened awareness of CVD risk leads to fear-related behaviors and physiological responses that promote systemic inflammation or endothelial reactivity, downstream effects on CRP levels might be observed. We measured CRP and VEGF at baseline only. Although this provides strong evidence that these biomarker-related phenomena do not confound the association between comparative optimism and CVD mortality, longitudinal assessment is necessary to determine the degree to which changes in systemic inflammation or endothelial dysregulation represents a causal pathway linking CVD risk perceptions and CVD death.

A better understanding of the substantial cardio-protective association between optimistic risk perceptions and cardiovascular health is essential to guide the preventive applications for emerging technologies (e.g. advanced imaging, predictive genomics) that will shape our society's understanding of cardiovascular risk.

Abbreviations

CRP:

C-reactive protein

VEGF:

vascular endothelial growth factor

ICD:

International Classification of Disease

NDI:

National Death Index

References

  1. Kok FJ, Matroos AW, van den Ban AW, Hautvast JG: Characteristics of individuals with multiple behavioral risk factors for coronary heart disease: the Netherlands. Am J Public Health. 1982, 72 (9): 986-991. 10.2105/AJPH.72.9.986.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Niknian M, McKinlay SM, Rakowski W, Carleton RA: A comparison of perceived and objective CVD risk in a general population. Am J Public Health. 1989, 79 (12): 1653-1654. 10.2105/AJPH.79.12.1653.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Avis NE, Smith KW, McKinlay JB: Accuracy of perceptions of heart attack risk: what influences perceptions and can they be changed?. Am J Public Health. 1989, 79 (12): 1608-1612. 10.2105/AJPH.79.12.1608.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Green JS, Grant M, Hill KL, Brizzolara J, Belmont B: Heart disease risk perception in college men and women. J Am Coll Health. 2003, 51 (5): 207-211. 10.1080/07448480309596352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Dearborn JL, McCullough LD: Perception of Risk and Knowledge of Risk Factors in Women at High Risk for Stroke. Stroke. 2009, 40 (4): 1181-6. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.543272.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Homko CJ, Santamore WP, Zamora L, Shirk G, Gaughan J, Cross R, Kashem A, Petersen S, Bove AA: Cardiovascular disease knowledge and risk perception among underserved individuals at increased risk of cardiovascular disease. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2008, 23 (4): 332-337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Marx JJ, Gube C, Faldum A, Kuntze H, Nedelmann M, Haertle B, Dieterich M, Eicke BM: An educational multimedia campaign improves stroke knowledge and risk perception in different stroke risk groups. Eur J Neurol. 2009

    Google Scholar 

  8. Eaker ED, Pinsky J, Castelli WP: Myocardial infarction and coronary death among women: psychosocial predictors from a 20-year follow-up of women in the Framingham Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1992, 135 (8): 854-864.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gramling R, Klein W, Roberts M, Waring ME, Gramling D, Eaton CB: Self-rated cardiovascular risk and 15-year cardiovascular mortality. Ann Fam Med. 2008, 6 (4): 302-306. 10.1370/afm.859.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. McKenney JL, Lapane KL, Assaf AR, Carleton RA: The association between perceived risk and actual cardiovascular disease. Epidemiology. 1995, 6 (6): 612-616. 10.1097/00001648-199511000-00009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Albert CM, Ma J, Rifai N, Stampfer MJ, Ridker PM: Prospective study of C-reactive protein, homocysteine, and plasma lipid levels as predictors of sudden cardiac death. Circulation. 2002, 105 (22): 2595-2599. 10.1161/01.CIR.0000017493.03108.1C.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ridker PM, Buring JE, Shih J, Matias M, Hennekens CH: Prospective study of C-reactive protein and the risk of future cardiovascular events among apparently healthy women. Circulation. 1998, 98 (8): 731-733.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ridker PM, Hennekens CH, Buring JE, Rifai N: C-reactive protein and other markers of inflammation in the prediction of cardiovascular disease in women. N Engl J Med. 2000, 342 (12): 836-843. 10.1056/NEJM200003233421202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Ridker PM, Rifai N, Cook NR, Bradwin G, Buring JE: Non-HDL cholesterol, apolipoproteins A-I and B100, standard lipid measures, lipid ratios, and CRP as risk factors for cardiovascular disease in women. Jama. 2005, 294 (3): 326-333. 10.1001/jama.294.3.326.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Ridker PM, Paynter NP, Rifai N, Gaziano JM, Cook NR: C-reactive protein and parental history improve global cardiovascular risk prediction: the Reynolds Risk Score for men. Circulation. 2008, 118 (22): 2243-2251. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.814251. 2244p following 2251

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Eaton CB, Gramling R, Parker DR, Roberts MB, Lu B, Ridker PM: Prospective association of vascular endothelial growth factor-A (VEGF-A) with coronary heart disease mortality in southeastern New England. Atherosclerosis. 2008, 200 (1): 221-227. 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2007.12.027.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Carleton RA, Lasater TM, Assaf AR, Feldman HA, McKinlay S: The Pawtucket Heart Health Program: community changes in cardiovascular risk factors and projected disease risk. Am J Public Health. 1995, 85 (6): 777-785. 10.2105/AJPH.85.6.777.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. D'Agostino RB: Propensity score methods for bias reduction in the comparison of a treatment to a non-randomized control group. Stat Med. 1998, 17 (19): 2265-2281. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19981015)17:19<2265::AID-SIM918>3.0.CO;2-B.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Scheier MF, Matthews KA, Owens JF, Magovern GJ, Lefebvre RC, Abbott RA, Carver CS: Dispositional optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: the beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989, 57 (6): 1024-1040. 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1024.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Scheier MF, Matthews KA, Owens JF, Schulz R, Bridges MW, Magovern GJ, Carver CS: Optimism and rehospitalization after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Arch Intern Med. 1999, 159 (8): 829-835. 10.1001/archinte.159.8.829.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Scheier MF, Carver CS: Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1992, 16: 201-228. 10.1007/BF01173489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Steptoe A, Wardle J, Vinck J, Tuomisto M, Holte A, Wichstrom L: Personality and attitudinal correlates of health lifestyles in young adults. Psychology and Health. 1994, 9: 331-343. 10.1080/08870449408407492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Shepperd JA, Moroto JJ, Pbert LA: Dispositional optimism as a predictor of health changes among cardiac patients. Journal of research and personality. 1996, 30: 517-534. 10.1006/jrpe.1996.0038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Klein WMP, Zajac LE: Imagining a rosy future: the psychology of optimism. Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. Edited by: Markman K, Klein WMP, J S. 2008, New York, NY: Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  25. Fiscella K: Self-Rated Risk versus Self-Rated Health. Annals of Family Medicine. 2008, 6-Accessed September 22, 2009, [http://www.annfammed.org/cgi/eletters/6/4/302]

    Google Scholar 

  26. Idler EL, Benyamini Y: Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav. 1997, 38 (1): 21-37. 10.2307/2955359.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Unden AL, Andreasson A, Elofsson S, Brismar K, Mathsson L, Ronnelid J, Lekander M: Inflammatory cytokines, behaviour and age as determinants of self-rated health in women. Clin Sci (Lond). 2007, 112 (6): 363-373. 10.1042/CS20060128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Janszky I, Lekander M, Blom M, Georgiades A, Ahnve S: Self-rated health and vital exhaustion, but not depression, is related to inflammation in women with coronary heart disease. Brain Behav Immun. 2005, 19 (6): 555-563. 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.01.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (National Institute for Nursing Research 1R03NR010409 & National Human Genome Research Institute 1R03HG004158), The Rhode Island Foundation and the Brown University Salomon Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Gramling.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

All authors participated in the interpretation of these findings, contributed substantively to the writing of this manuscript and approved this submission. RG, MR, and CB participated in data collection and initial analyses. WK, LZ, and KH participated in subsequent revised analyses.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gramling, R., Heffner, K.L., Klein, W.M. et al. Is the association between optimistic cardiovascular risk perceptions and lower rates of cardiovascular disease mortality explained by biomarkers of systemic inflammation or endothelial function? A case-cohort study. BioPsychoSocial Med 4, 11 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-4-11

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-4-11

Keywords